June 29, 2020 Update:
The coronavirus crisis has called attention to economic inequality in new ways, with data suggesting that the less well-off are more likely than others to become infected. Not only that, but experts say the inequalities may also put the rest of society at risk, because lower-income families (with no paid sick days, for example) are often 鈥渇orced to accept a higher risk of exposure that can infect others,鈥听.听Making the case for greater economic equality is and will remain a challenge in American politics, notes Carroll School marketing professor Nailya Ordabayeva. She has conducted an array of studies and experiments revealing how different messages in favor of income redistribution may fly, or not fly, with different segments of the public. What follows is an account of this recent research (another article of hers, on post-pandemic economic restructuring, appeared recently in听).
People have questions about rising economic inequality鈥攚hether it鈥檚 fair and what sorts of policies would lessen the disparities. As a marketing professor, Nailya Ordabayeva of 艾可直播 College鈥檚 Carroll School of Management has a particular question: How do you sell these policies to the American public?
Recent research has suggested some broad avenues. For example, people who feel they have much in common with their fellow Americans are more likely to back measures aimed at reducing inequality through a downward redistribution of income, according to a cluster of studies. The findings have led researchers to expect that messages emphasizing similarities among people will tend to sway minds toward support for such policies.
But something has gone unnoticed in the research, according to Ordabayeva鈥檚 studies.
Messages about how we鈥檙e all in the same boat do tend to work well among people more liberal in their politics. In her clinical experiments, when liberals hear those pitches, their support for income redistribution goes up. But these same messages lead conservatives to tamp down any enthusiasm they might have for spreading the wealth. That鈥檚 the missing piece in previous research.
The upshot: If you鈥檙e on the stump for policies that might narrow gaps between rich and poor, you need to craft different messages for different groups along the ideological spectrum. In fact, the one-size-fits-all approach will often 鈥渂ackfire and further polarize, rather than unite, people鈥檚 views about redistributive policies,鈥 Ordabayeva says.
In other words, messages that accent commonality will help win over those who lean left (thus advancing policies such as heftier taxes on the very wealthy), but may repel those who lean toward the right. Why?
For liberals, the messages boost their beliefs that people 鈥渨ork similarly hard and are similarly talented, so they should be similarly rewarded,鈥 Ordabayeva points out. Conservatives, however, typically believe that people are different when it comes to talent and effort, and for that reason, some deserve success more than others. So, when they hear about how we're alike, what they think is that Americans have 鈥渟imilar opportunities and starting conditions to succeed, and those who don鈥檛 succeed have only themselves to blame.鈥
鈥淒issimilar鈥 messages might work with conservatives
Ordabayeva鈥檚 paper, 鈥,鈥 appeared last year in the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. It drew on a series of studies involving more than 1,500 Americans. A previous article for that journal, titled and coauthored with Daniel Fernandes of the Catholic University of Portugal, reported on another 1,600 studies.
In the various experiments, participants have responded to questions and statements such as 鈥淭he people I identify with are a lot different from most Americans.鈥 In one study, people were prompted to describe how similar or dissimilar they felt toward others, and the experiment led them to feel greater similarity than those who did not receive the prompt. This, in turn, made liberals more disposed toward policies intended to promote greater economic equality. Conservatives became less disposed.
If appeals to similarity don鈥檛 work for conservatives, what messages do? The answer, according to Ordabayeva, is to talk with this group about 鈥渄issimilarity.鈥
Indeed, conservatives became more likely to support redistribution when they thought that they were less similar to others. Ordabayeva explains that messages about dissimilarity may nudge conservatives to consider that people have 鈥渦nequal opportunities in life and that some people face better conditions for success than others,鈥 boosting their support for redistribution. Asked for an example of a message or prompt that may swing conservatives rather than liberals, she mentioned, 鈥淎mericans are different from each other in many ways,鈥 and 鈥淩emember how we are all different.鈥
Overall, the picture is more layered and complex than recent research has suggested, according to Ordabayeva. 鈥淭he findings,鈥 she wrote in her 2019 paper, 鈥渟uggest that messages designed to shift public support for redistribution by changing perceptions of social similarity would be most effective if tailored to the audience.鈥
William Bole is director of content development at the Carroll School.